확신이 없는 객관식 답 바꿔야할까?
게시글 주소: https://orbi.kr/00061056527
밑에 영어는 책에서 퍼온 건데 좀 더 자세한 얘기가 있어요
결론하고 알아두면 좋아보이는 부분만 제가 볼드체로 바꿨어요
Across various surveys, large proportions—between 68% and 100%—of college students say that changing their initial answers on a test won’t improve their score. About three fourths say that chang- ing their answers will actually lower their score (Ballance, 1977; Benjamin, Cavell, & Shallenberger, 1984). This myth—sometimes called the “first instinct fallacy”—isn’t limited to undergraduates. In one study, among professors who gave their college students advice about changing answers on tests, 63% told them not to do so because it would tend to lower their scores. Among science and liberal arts professors, only 5–6% said that changing answers would tend to increase students’ scores; the per- centage among education professors was 30% (Benjamin et al., 1984). What’s more, scores of websites, including those designed to provide students with test-taking advice, inform readers that changing their initial answers is a bad strategy and encourage them to trust their first hunches. One website tells students, “Don’t keep on changing your answer—usually your first choice is the right one, unless you misread the question” (TestTakingTips.com) and another advises them to “Trust your first hunch. When you answer a question, go with your first hunch —don’t change your answer unless you’re absolutely sure you’re right” (Tomahawk Elementary School). Another goes further, even citing research support for this belief: “Be wary of changing your mind: There is evidence to suggest that students more frequently change right answers to wrong ones than wrong answers to right ones” (Fetzner Student-Athlete Academic Center).
What do the scientific findings actually say? With over 3 million high school students taking the SAT and ACT (interestingly, in the case of both tests the letters don’t stand for anything) each year, this question is hardly trivial. In fact, the research evidence is surprisingly consistent, and it points to the opposite conclusion presented on these websites (Benjamin et al., 1984; Geiger, 1996; Skinner, 1983; Waddell & Blankenship, 1994). More than 60 studies lead to essentially the same verdict: When students change answers on multiple-choice tests (typically as judged by their erasures or cross-outs of earlier answers), they’re more likely to change from a wrong to a right answer than from a right to a wrong answer. For each point that students lose when changing from a right to a wrong answer, they gain between two and three points on average in changing from a wrong to a right answer (Benjamin et al., 1984; Foote & Belinky, 1972; Geiger, 1996). In addi- tion, students who change more answers tend to receive higher test scores than other students, although this finding is only correlational (see Intro- duction, p. 13) and may reflect the fact that frequent answer-changers are higher test performers to begin with (Geiger, 1997; Friedman & Cook, 1995). All of these conclusions hold not merely for multiple choice tests given in classes, but for standardized tests like the SAT and Graduate Record Exam (GRE).
Admittedly, there are two qualifications to the “when in doubt, change your answer” strategy. First, research suggests that students shouldn’t change their answer if they’re merely guessing this answer might be wrong; changing one’s answer is beneficial only when students have a good reason to suspect their answer is wrong (Shatz & Best, 1987; Skinner,1983). Second, there’s some evidence that students who do poorly on multiple choice tests may benefit less from changing their answers than other students (Best, 1979). So these students may want to change their answers only when they’re fairly certain these answers are wrong.
There’s surprisingly little research addressing the question of why students believe that changing their initial answers is usually a bad idea. But three likely explanations come to mind. First, as we’ve seen, most professors who give their students advice about changing their answers advise them not to do so (Benjamin et al., 1984). So this mistaken belief is probably spread partly by word-of-mouth (Higham & Gerrard, 2005). Second, research suggests that students are more likely to remember items whose answers they changed from right to wrong than those they changed from wrong to right (Bath, 1967; Ferguson, Kreiter, Peterson, Rowat, & Elliott, 2002). Because the bitter taste of incorrect decisions lingers longer than the memory of correct decisions (“Why on earth did I change that answer? I had it right the first time”), our test-taking mistakes typically stick in our minds. As a consequence, a phenomenon called the availability heuristic may lead students to overestimate the risk of committing errors when changing answers. As we learned earlier (see Introduction, p. 15), a heuristic is a mental shortcut or rule of thumb. When we use the availability heuristic, we’re estimating the likelihood of an event by how easily it comes to our minds. Indeed, research shows that students who change right answers to wrong answers recall these decisions much better than do students who change wrong answers to right answers, largely because the former changes create a more last- ing emotional impact (Kruger, Wirtz, & Miller, 2005). Third, research indicates that most students overestimate how many answers they get right on multiple choice tests (Pressley & Ghatala, 1988), so they may assume that changing answers is likely to lower their score.
So to cut to the bottom line: When in doubt, we’re usually best not trusting our instincts. After all, our first hunches are just that—hunches. If we have a good reason to believe we’re wrong, we should go with our head, not our gut, and turn that pencil upside-down.
50 Great Myths of Popular Psychology Shattering Widespread Misconceptions about Human Behavior by Scott O. Lilienfeld Steven Jay Lynn John Ruscio Barry L. Beyerstein
결론: 단순히 찝찝하다는 느낌을 넘어섰을 때 바꿔야 좋다.
0 XDK (+0)
유익한 글을 읽었다면 작성자에게 XDK를 선물하세요.
-
사람들이 날 글케 안싫어하나바ㅎ
-
지구가 4...... 이번 지구 나만 어려웠구나...
-
될까.....
-
자대배치 집가까운곳 제발요
-
저분이 드립력 고트인듯 벽느껴짐
-
나만차단많이하나 9
오르비하면서화나고싶지않아
-
맞는지 모르겎음 제일 팔팔할때 일하고 공부하고 어디하나 불편해지면 편하게 사는것보다...
-
이 시간대 새끼들은 다 나랑 친구였구나 반갑다 야
-
차단목록 ㅇㅈ 4
-
왜 더프 성적표가 안나오는 거야 하.... 분명 1주일 전만 해도 3월 성적표는...
-
인증합니다. 3
차단목록 인증합니다. 0명이니 모두 안심하세요.
-
제 전글에 모아놨으니 쉽게 차단하실수 있어요 !
-
아 뭐야 방송 안꺼졌네
-
차단목록 ㅇㅈ 2
차단하기 귀찮음
-
ㅋㅋ 7
ㅋㅋ
-
오르비 애니프사들 집합 17
애니추천좀 해주세요 !
-
내옷 어제 꺼낸건데 엄마가 점심에 빨앗대서 빡쳣는데 지금 만져보니까 다 말랏음
-
1-1번부터 숨이 턱
-
차단목록 ㅇㅈ 6
날 싫어하는 사람은 많을지언정 누구를 싫어하진 않음
-
국어 포함 아무 주제이든 좋습니다
-
전남친목방 1
나가 쇼
-
이러면 걍 한국에 있는 대학을 가지
-
저 왔어요^^ 0
안녕히계세요^^
-
내차단목록까면 10
상처받을사람들많을듯
-
옯붕이들도 다들 행복한 4월되십쇼 경희대 국제캠
-
오르비 잘자요
-
중딩 때 수학 진도 미적까지 쫙 빼면 어떨지 궁금하긴함
-
내엠비티아이맞춰보셈 18
너무쉬울걸
-
1~30 중 내가 생각중인 숫자 맞히면 3천덕 드림 16
ㄱㄱ 한명만!
-
외대가진짜 2
이쁜사람많더라
-
??
-
님들아..저 생각보다 계획적이에요
-
불리함을 마다하고 한번 열어봐야지
-
n티켓 시즌1 다 끝나가여 공통!!
-
이 나이에도 수요가 없는데 30되면 더 옶지 않을까 싶어서 슬퍼짐
-
231114 내용 자체가 엄청 어렵고 그런건 아녔구나 15
시험장에서 1번 찍기가 망설여지긴 했을듯..
-
동의동의ㅠㅠ
-
ㅈ망게임
-
졸업은 할 수 있을까
-
[자작시] 오나홀 10
좋은 대학을 가서 돈을 많이 벌어야 예쁜 여친을 만날 수 있다 하셨습니다. 그런...
-
선착순 한명 옛다 5천덕
-
재능있는 내친구 일회독 6시간만에 끝나는걸 나는 일주일동안 일회독하고 문제도 잘...
-
수1 앞부분에 지수로그 실생활 활용인가 뭐 그런문제가 수십개인데 이것조차도 풀어봐야할까요...?
-
차단목록메타열기 2
-
개꿀잼일텐데
-
과탐 선택이 1
잘못된 선택이 아니길...
-
나도 살 빼야됨 2
12kg찐거 정상화해야됨
-
솔직히 얜 좀 구리죠 든든하게 얘 사는게 맞아
응 바꿔서 수학 4점 날렸어~
ㅋㅋㅋㅋ
근데 고쳐서 틀린게 한두개가 아님